英语流利说 Level7 Unit3 Part2 Learning: On Reading Minds

Rebecca Saxe :On Reading Minds | TED Talk

Video 1: On Reading Minds I

Today I'm going to talk to you about the problem of other minds.

And the problem I'm going to talk about is not the familiar one from philosophy, which is, "How can we know whether other people have minds?"

That is, maybe you have a mind, and everyone else is just a really convincing robot.

So that's a problem in philosophy, but for today's purposes I'm going to assume that many people in this audience have a mind, and that I don't have to worry about this.

There is a second problem that is maybe even more familiar to us as parents and teachers and spouses and novelists,

which is, "Why is it so hard to know what somebody else wants or believes?"

Or perhaps, more relevantly, "Why is it so hard to change what somebody else wants or believes?"

I think novelists put this best.

Like Philip Roth, who said, "And yet, what are we to do about this terribly significant business of other people?

So ill equipped are we all, to envision one another's interior workings and invisible aims."

So as a teacher and as a spouse, this is, of course, a problem I confront every day.

But as a scientist, I'm interested in a different problem of other minds, and that is the one I'm going to introduce to you today.

And that problem is, "How is it so easy to know other minds?"

So to start with an illustration, you need almost no information, one snapshot of a stranger, to guess what this woman is thinking, or what this man is.

And put another way, the crux of the problem is the machine that we use for thinking about other minds, our brain, is made up of pieces, brain cells,

that we share with all other animals, with monkeys and mice and even sea slugs.

And yet, you put them together in a particular network, and what you get is the capacity to write Romeo and Juliet.

Or to say, as Alan Greenspan did, "I know you think you understand what you thought I said, but I'm not sure you realize that what you heard is not what I meant." (Laughter)

Which of the following is a common topic of discussion in philosophy? Whether or not other people are conscious

"So ill equipped are we all, to envision one another's interior workings and invisible aims" in this context what does "one interior workings" refer to? how one's mind works

The crux of the problem is the most important or difficult part of it.


Video 2: On Reading Minds II

So, the job of my field of cognitive neuroscience is to stand with these ideas, one in each hand.

And to try to understand how you can put together simple units, simple messages over space and time, in a network,

and get this amazing human capacity to think about minds.

So I'm going to tell you three things about this today.

Obviously the whole project here is huge.

And I'm going to tell you just our first few steps about the discovery of a special brain region for thinking about other people's thoughts.

Some observations on the slow development of this system as we learn how to do this difficult job.

And then finally, to show that some of the differences between people, in how we judge others, can be explained by differences in this brain system.

So first, the first thing I want to tell you is that there is a brain region in the human brain, in your brains, whose job it is to think about other people's thoughts.

This is a picture of it.

It's called the Right Temporo-Parietal Junction. It's above and behind your right ear.

And this is the brain region you used when you saw the pictures I showed you, or when you read Romeo and Juliet or when you tried to understand Alan Greenspan.

And you don't use it for solving any other kinds of logical problems.

So this brain region is called the Right TPJ.

And this picture shows the average activation in a group of what we call typical human adults.

They're MIT undergraduates. (Laughter)

The second thing I want to say about this brain system is that although we human adults are really good at understanding other minds, we weren't always that way.

It takes children a long time to break into the system.

I'm going to show you a little bit of that long, extended process.

The first thing I'm going to show you is a change between age three and five, as kids learn to understand that somebody else can have beliefs that are different from their own.

So I'm going to show you a five-year-old who is getting a standard kind of puzzle that we call the false belief task.


What do "cognitive neuroscience" like Saxe's study ? how the brain constructs thoughts


Rebecca Saxe (Video): This is the first pirate. His name is Ivan. And you know what pirates really like?

Child: What? RS: Pirates really like cheese sandwiches.

Child: Cheese? I love cheese! RS: Yeah.

So Ivan has this cheese sandwich, and he says, "Yum yum yum yum yum! I really love cheese sandwiches."

And Ivan puts his sandwich over here, on top of the pirate chest.

And Ivan says, "You know what? I need a drink with my lunch." And so Ivan goes to get a drink.

And while Ivan is away the wind comes, and it blows the sandwich down onto the grass.

And now, here comes the other pirate. This pirate is called Joshua.

And Joshua also really loves cheese sandwiches. So Joshua has a cheese sandwich and he says, "Yum yum yum yum yum! I love cheese sandwiches."

And he puts his cheese sandwich over here on top of the pirate chest.

Child: So, that one is his.RS: That one is Joshua's. That's right.Child: And then his went on the ground.

RS: That's exactly right.Child: So he won't know which one is his.RS: Oh. So now Joshua goes off to get a drink.

Ivan comes back and he says, "I want my cheese sandwich." So which one do you think Ivan is going to take?

Child: I think he is going to take that one.RS: Yeah, you think he's going to take that one? All right. Let's see.

Oh yeah, you were right. He took that one.

So that's a five-year-old who clearly understands that other people can have false beliefs and what the consequences are for their actions.

Now I'm going to show you a three-year-old who got the same puzzle.

RS: And Ivan says, "I want my cheese sandwich." Which sandwich is he going to take?

Do you think he's going to take that one? Let's see what happens. Let's see what he does. Here comes Ivan. And he says, "I want my cheese sandwich." And he takes this one.

Uh-oh. Why did he take that one? Child: His was on the grass.

So the three-year-old does two things differently.

First, he predicts Ivan will take the sandwich that's really his.

And second, when he sees Ivan taking the sandwich where he left his, where we would say he's taking that one because he thinks it's his,

the three-year-old comes up with another explanation: He's not taking his own sandwich because he doesn't want it, because now it's dirty, on the ground.

So that's why he's taking the other sandwich.

Now of course, development doesn't end at five.

And we can see the continuation of this process of learning to think about other people's thoughts by upping the ante and asking children now, not for an action prediction, but for a moral judgment.

So first I'm going to show you the three-year-old again.

RS.: So is Ivan being mean and naughty for taking Joshua's sandwich? Child: Yeah.

RS: Should Ivan get in trouble for taking Joshua's sandwich? Child: Yeah.

So it's maybe not surprising he thinks it was mean of Ivan to take Joshua's sandwich,

since he thinks Ivan only took Joshua's sandwich to avoid having to eat his own dirty sandwich.

But now I'm going to show you the five-year-old.

Remember the five-year-old completely understood why Ivan took Joshua's sandwich.

RS: Was Ivan being mean and naughty for taking Joshua's sandwich?

Child: Um, yeah.

And so, it is not until age seven that we get what looks more like an adult response.

RS: Should Ivan get in trouble for taking Joshua's sandwich?

Child: No, because the wind should get in trouble.

He says the wind should get in trouble for switching the sandwiches. (Laughter)


What did Saxe learn from the experiment? The saven-year-old demonstrated an adult-like moral judgment.

Which of the statements about the three boys' experiment is true? The 7-year-old had developed a sense of moral judgment similar to an adult's.

Which of the following is a moral judgment?he is a mean and selfish man

You don't use the RTPJ for solving any other kinds of logical problems.


Video 3: On Reading Minds III

And now what we've started to do in my lab is to put children into the brain scanner and ask what's going on in their brain as they develop this ability to think about other people's thoughts.

So the first thing is that in children we see this same brain region, the Right TPJ, being used while children are thinking about other people.

But it's not quite like the adult brain.

So whereas in the adults, as I told you, this brain region is almost completely specialized -- it does almost nothing else except for thinking about other people's thoughts --

in children it's much less so, when they are age five to eight, the age range of the children I just showed you.

And actually if we even look at eight to 11-year-olds, getting into early adolescence, they still don't have quite an adult-like brain region.

And so, what we can see is that over the course of childhood and even into adolescence,

both the cognitive system, our mind's ability to think about other minds,and the brain system that supports it are continuing, slowly, to develop.

But of course, as you're probably aware, even in adulthood, people differ from one another in how good they are at thinking of other minds, how often they do it and how accurately.

And so what we wanted to know was, could differences among adults in how they think about other people's thoughts be explained in terms of differences in this brain region?

So, the first thing that we did is we gave adults a version of the pirate problem that we gave to the kids. And I'm going to give that to you now.


What's the purpose of the Experiment for adults ? to see what is responsible for the differences in adults' ability to envision other people's thoughts

If something is specialized, it is developed for a specific purpose.


So Grace and her friend are on a tour of a chemical factory, and they take a break for coffee. And Grace's friend asks for some sugar in her coffee.

Grace goes to make the coffee and finds by the coffee a pot containing a white powder, which is sugar.

But the powder is labeled "Deadly Poison," so Grace thinks that the powder is a deadly poison.

And she puts it in her friend's coffee. And her friend drinks the coffee, and is fine.

How many people think it was morally permissible for Grace to put the powder in the coffee?

Okay. Good. (Laugh.ter)

So we ask people, how much should Grace be blamed in this case, which we call a failed attempt to harm?

And we can compare that to another case, where everything in the real world is the same. The powder is still sugar, but what's different is what Grace thinks.

Now she thinks the powder is sugar.

And perhaps unsurprisingly, if Grace thinks the powder is sugar and puts it in her friend's coffee, people say she deserves no blame at all.

Whereas if she thinks the powder was poison, even though it's really sugar, now people say she deserves a lot of blame,

even though what happened in the real world was exactly the same.

And in fact, they say she deserves more blame in this case, the failed attempt to harm, than in another case, which we call an accident.

Where Grace thought the powder was sugar, because it was labeled "sugar" and by the coffee machine, but actually the powder was poison.

So even though when the powder was poison, the friend drank the coffee and died,

people say Grace deserves less blame in that case, when she innocently thought it was sugar, than in the other case, where she thought it was poison and no harm occurred.

People, though, disagree a little bit about exactly how much blame Grace should get in the accident case.

Some people think she should deserve more blame, and other people less.

And what I'm going to show you is what happened when we look inside the brains of people while they're making that judgment.

So what I'm showing you, from left to right, is how much activity there was in this brain region, and from top to bottom, how much blame people said that Grace deserved.

And what you can see is, on the left when there was very little activity in this brain region, people paid little attention to her innocent belief and said she deserved a lot of blame for the accident.

Where as on the right, where there was a lot of activity, people paid a lot more attention to her innocent belief, and said she deserved a lot less blame for causing the accident.


Which of the following is failed attempt to harm? Grace knew mistook sugar for poison, put it into her friend's coffee, and caused no harm.

Which two available influenced people's opinions of Grace? her belief and behavior

if something is morally permissible it is acceptable according to the principles of good behavior.


Video 4: On Reading Minds IV

So that's good, but of course what we'd rather is have a way to interfere with function in this brain region, and see if we could change people's moral judgment.

And we do have such a tool.

It's called Trans-Cranial Magnetic Stimulation, or TMS.

This is a tool that lets us pass a magnetic pulse through somebody's skull, into a small region of their brain, and temporarily disorganize the function of the neurons in that region.

So I'm going to show you a demo of this.

First, I'm going to show you that this is a magnetic pulse.

I'm going to show you what happens when you put a quarter on the machine. When you hear clicks, we're turning the machine on.

So now I'm going to apply that same pulse to my brain, to the part of my brain that controls my hand.

So there is no physical force, just a magnetic pulse.

Okay, so it causes a small involuntary contraction in my hand by putting a magnetic pulse in my brain.

And we can use that same pulse, now applied to the RTPJ, to ask if we can change people's moral judgments.

So these are the judgments I showed you before, people's normal moral judgments.

And then we can apply TMS to the RTPJ and ask how people's judgments change.

And the first thing is, people can still do this task overall.

So their judgments of the case when everything was fine remain the same. They say she deserves no blame.

But in the case of a failed attempt to harm, where Grace thought that it was poison, although it was really sugar,

people now say it was more okay, she deserves less blame for putting the powder in the coffee.

And in the case of the accident, where she thought that it was sugar, but it was really poison and so she caused a death,

people say that it was less okay, she deserves more blame.

So what I've told you today is that people come, actually, especially well equipped to think about other people's thoughts.

We have a special brain system that lets us think about what other people are thinking.

This system takes a long time to develop, slowly throughout the course of childhood and into early adolescence.

And even in adulthood, differences in this brain region can explain differences among adults in how we think about and judge other people.

But I want to give the last word back to the novelists, and to Philip Roth, who ended by saying,

"The fact remains that getting people right is not what living is all about anyway. It's getting them wrong that is living.

Getting them wrong and wrong and wrong, and then on careful reconsideration, getting them wrong again." Thank you.(Applause)


Why did Saxe apply a magnetic pulse to the  RTPJ ? to see if it's possible to change people's moral judgments

In the demonstration Trans-Cranial Magnetic Stimulation is a tool to disorganize the function of neurons in the RTPJ.

She conducts the experiment to see if it's  possible to change people's  moral judgments.


GAMES-Grammar

1. What does cognitive neuroscience focus on? How the human brain constructs thoughts

2. The crux of a problem is the most  important part of it.

3.puzzle perplex infuse confuse reduce

4.But of course, as you're probably aware, even in adulthood, people differ from one another in how good they are at thinking of other minds, how often they do it and how accurately. And so what we wanted to know was, could differences among adults in how they think about other people's thoughts be explained in terms of differences in this brain region?

5.

1)Grace and her friend are on a tour of a chemical factory, and they take a break for coffee. 

2)And Grace's friend asks for some sugar in her coffee. 

3)Grace goes to make the coffee and finds by the coffee a pot containing a white powder, which is sugar. 

4)But the powder is labeled "Deadly Poison," so Grace thinks that the powder is a deadly poison. 

6.The second thing I want to say about this brain system is that although we human adults are really good at understanding other minds, we weren't always that way. 

It takes children a long time to break into the system. 

The first thing I'm going to show you is a change between age three and five.

During this period, kids learn to understand that somebody else can have beliefs that are different from their own.

GAMES-Speaking

1.If something is specialized, it is designed or develop for a specific purpose.

2.Some people think she should deserve more blame, and other people think she should deserve less.

3.The five-year-old understood why Ivan would mistake the sandwich as his.

4.If something is morally permissible, it is acceptable according to the principles of good behavior.

5.We have a special brain system that lets us think about what other people are thinking.

最后编辑于
©著作权归作者所有,转载或内容合作请联系作者
  • 序言:七十年代末,一起剥皮案震惊了整个滨河市,随后出现的几起案子,更是在滨河造成了极大的恐慌,老刑警刘岩,带你破解...
    沈念sama阅读 204,530评论 6 478
  • 序言:滨河连续发生了三起死亡事件,死亡现场离奇诡异,居然都是意外死亡,警方通过查阅死者的电脑和手机,发现死者居然都...
    沈念sama阅读 86,403评论 2 381
  • 文/潘晓璐 我一进店门,熙熙楼的掌柜王于贵愁眉苦脸地迎上来,“玉大人,你说我怎么就摊上这事。” “怎么了?”我有些...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 151,120评论 0 337
  • 文/不坏的土叔 我叫张陵,是天一观的道长。 经常有香客问我,道长,这世上最难降的妖魔是什么? 我笑而不...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 54,770评论 1 277
  • 正文 为了忘掉前任,我火速办了婚礼,结果婚礼上,老公的妹妹穿的比我还像新娘。我一直安慰自己,他们只是感情好,可当我...
    茶点故事阅读 63,758评论 5 367
  • 文/花漫 我一把揭开白布。 她就那样静静地躺着,像睡着了一般。 火红的嫁衣衬着肌肤如雪。 梳的纹丝不乱的头发上,一...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 48,649评论 1 281
  • 那天,我揣着相机与录音,去河边找鬼。 笑死,一个胖子当着我的面吹牛,可吹牛的内容都是我干的。 我是一名探鬼主播,决...
    沈念sama阅读 38,021评论 3 398
  • 文/苍兰香墨 我猛地睁开眼,长吁一口气:“原来是场噩梦啊……” “哼!你这毒妇竟也来了?” 一声冷哼从身侧响起,我...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 36,675评论 0 258
  • 序言:老挝万荣一对情侣失踪,失踪者是张志新(化名)和其女友刘颖,没想到半个月后,有当地人在树林里发现了一具尸体,经...
    沈念sama阅读 40,931评论 1 299
  • 正文 独居荒郊野岭守林人离奇死亡,尸身上长有42处带血的脓包…… 初始之章·张勋 以下内容为张勋视角 年9月15日...
    茶点故事阅读 35,659评论 2 321
  • 正文 我和宋清朗相恋三年,在试婚纱的时候发现自己被绿了。 大学时的朋友给我发了我未婚夫和他白月光在一起吃饭的照片。...
    茶点故事阅读 37,751评论 1 330
  • 序言:一个原本活蹦乱跳的男人离奇死亡,死状恐怖,灵堂内的尸体忽然破棺而出,到底是诈尸还是另有隐情,我是刑警宁泽,带...
    沈念sama阅读 33,410评论 4 321
  • 正文 年R本政府宣布,位于F岛的核电站,受9级特大地震影响,放射性物质发生泄漏。R本人自食恶果不足惜,却给世界环境...
    茶点故事阅读 39,004评论 3 307
  • 文/蒙蒙 一、第九天 我趴在偏房一处隐蔽的房顶上张望。 院中可真热闹,春花似锦、人声如沸。这庄子的主人今日做“春日...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 29,969评论 0 19
  • 文/苍兰香墨 我抬头看了看天上的太阳。三九已至,却和暖如春,着一层夹袄步出监牢的瞬间,已是汗流浃背。 一阵脚步声响...
    开封第一讲书人阅读 31,203评论 1 260
  • 我被黑心中介骗来泰国打工, 没想到刚下飞机就差点儿被人妖公主榨干…… 1. 我叫王不留,地道东北人。 一个月前我还...
    沈念sama阅读 45,042评论 2 350
  • 正文 我出身青楼,却偏偏与公主长得像,于是被迫代替她去往敌国和亲。 传闻我的和亲对象是个残疾皇子,可洞房花烛夜当晚...
    茶点故事阅读 42,493评论 2 343

推荐阅读更多精彩内容

  • 每天下午将近下班时分,公司对面的马路牙子旁摆满了各色地边摊,逢着不忙的时候,下班去公司附近的一个学校操场跑步,...
    Hi栀子酱阅读 364评论 0 0